The Effects of Emotional Valence and Arousal in the Free Recall of Serbian Nouns
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.19090/pp.v18i3.2598Keywords:
emotional valence, arousal, concreteness, free recallAbstract
Recent studies demonstrated inconsistent patterns of results regarding emotional valence (EV) and arousal (A) in memorising words. According to the representational substitution hypothesis, emotions play a central role in representing abstract words, predicting a larger valence effect in the lexical processing of abstract words. Conversely, the multimodal induction hypothesis suggests that emotions may be more accessible for concrete words, as emotions can be readily evoked by activating relevant sensorimotor experiences. In our study, we tested these hypotheses through the incidental free recall task. During the implicit learning phase, 276 psychology students estimated different aspects (vividness of mental images associated with words, EV, and A) of the 64 Serbian nouns (32 concrete, 32 abstract). Concrete and abstract nouns were additionally divided into groups of positively and negatively valenced words that were either low or high in arousal. The stimuli were averaged for length, frequency, familiarity, age of acquisition, and context availability. The higher recall for positive words was recorded, regardless of concreteness, and for words with lower arousal. However, arousal had a distinct effect on abstract negative words, enhancing their recall. The concreteness effect was evident only for non-arousing negative words, highlighting the importance of sensory-based representations for less emotionally charged words during recall. These findings suggest that emotional experience plays a more critical role in representing abstract words, supporting the representational substitution hypothesis. However, since we did not include neutral words, we cannot completely reject the multimodal induction assumption.
References
Adelman, J. S., & Estes, Z. (2013). Emotion and memory: a recognition advantage for positive and negative words independent of arousal. Cognition, 129(3), 530–535. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.014 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.014
Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 578-602. https://doi.org/1.3758/BF03200738 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200738
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3). https://doi.org/1.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577–66. https://doi.org/1.1017/S0140525X99002149 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645. https://doi.org/1.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/1.18637/jss.v067.i01 DOI: https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Boucher, J., & Osgood, C. E. (1969). The Pollyanna hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80002-2 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80002-2
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Vol. 30, No. 1, pp. 25–36). Technical report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. https://e-lub.net/media/anew.pdf
Citron, M. M., Gray, M. A., Critchley, H. D., Weekes, B. S., & Ferstl, E. C. (2014a). Emotional valence and arousal affect reading in an interactive way: Neuroimaging evidence for an approach-withdrawal framework. Neuropsychologia, 56(100), 79–89. https://doi.org/1.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.002 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.002
Citron, F. M., Weekes, B. S., & Ferstl, E. C. (2014b). Arousal and emotional valence interact in written word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(10), 1257-1267. https://doi.org/1.1080/23273798.2014.897734 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.897734
Feldman Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(4), 967–984. https://doi.org/1.1037/0022-3514.74.4.967 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.4.967
Fliessbach, K., Weis, S., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2006). The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory. NeuroImage, 32(3), 1413–1421. https://doi.org/1.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.007
Ganis, G., & Kievit, R. A. (2015). A New Set of Three-Dimensional Shapes for Investigating Mental Rotation Processes: Validation Data and Stimulus Set. Journal of Open Psychology Data, 3(1), e3. https://doi.org/1.5334/jopd.ai DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.ai
Hale S. C. (1988). Spacetime and the abstract/concrete distinction. Philosophical Studies, 53, 85–102. https://doi.org/1.1007/BF00355677 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00355677
Jaeger T. F. (2008). Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of memory and language, 59(4), 434–446. https://doi.org/1.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
Janković, M., Vasović, S. & Popović Stijačić, M. (2023). The influence of word processing depth in implicit learning on reproduction performance, Scientific conference Current trends in psychology 2023, Faculty of Philosophy Novi Sad, October 26-28, Book of abstracts, p. 203-205. Avaliable at: https://empirijskaistrazivanja.org/wp-content/uploads/2023/03/EIP2023_book_of_abstracts.pdf
Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the Details: Effects of Emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99. https://doi.org/1.1177/1754073908100432 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908100432
Kleinsmith, L. J., & Kaplan, S. (1963). Paired-associate learning as a function of arousal and interpolated interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 190–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040288 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040288
Kousta, S. T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112(3), 473-481. https://doi.org/1.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14–34. https://doi.org/1.1037/a0021446 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021446
Leiner, D. J. (2021). SoSci Survey (Version 3.2.31) [Computer software]. Available at https://www.soscisurvey.de
Ljubešić, N. & Klubička, F. (2016). Serbian web corpus srWaC 1.1, Slovenian language resource repository CLARIN.SI, ISSN 2820-4042, http://hdl.handle.net/11356/1063.
Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 482–488. https://doi.org/1.1037/h0045106 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045106
Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002–1005. https://doi.org/1.1126/science.1136930 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1136930
Paivo, A. (1964). Generalization of verbally conditioned meaning from symbol to referent. Canadian Journal of Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie, 18(2), 146–155. https://doi.org/1.1037/h0083289 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083289
Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(1, Pt.2), 1–25. https://doi.org/1.1037/h0025327 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025327
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315798868 DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315798868
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford University Press.
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie, 45(3), 255–287. https://doi.org/1.1037/h0084295 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084295
Popović Stijačić, M. (2021). The relevance of perceptual information in word processing – the perspective of embodied cognition [Doctoral thesis, University of Novi Sad]. University of Novi Sad. Available at: https://www.cris.uns.ac.rs/DownloadFileServlet/Disertacija161762163851549.pdf?controlNumber=(BISIS)117683&fileName=161762163851549.pdf&id=17699&licenseAccepted=true
Popović Stijačić, M. Mihić, Lj. & Filipović Đurđević, D. (2018). Analyzing data from memory tasks - comparison of ANOVA, logistic regression and mixed logit model. Psihologija, 51(4), 469-488. https://doi.org/1.2298/PSI170615023P DOI: https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI170615023P
Popović Stijačić, M. & Filipović Đurđević, D. (2022). Perceptual richness of words and its role in free and cued recall. Primenjena Psihologija, 15(3), 355–381. https://doi.org/10.19090/pp.v15i3.2400 DOI: https://doi.org/10.19090/pp.v15i3.2400
Popović Stijačić, M., Mišić, K., & Filipović Đurđević, D. (2023). Flattening the curve: COVID-19 induced a decrease in arousal for positive and an increase in arousal for negative words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 44(6), 1069–1089. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716423000425 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716423000425
Popović Stijačić, M., & Protić, K. (2025, June 1). Effects of Arousal, Emotional valence and Concreteness in Incidental Free Recall: Data and R code. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/M7Z3F
Russell J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological review, 110(1), 145–172. https://doi.org/1.1037/0033-295x.11.1.145 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.110.1.145
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation
for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.
Strik Lievers, F., Bolognesi, M. & Winter, B. (2021). The linguistic dimensions of concrete and abstract concepts: lexical category, morphological structure, countability, and etymology. Cognitive Linguistics, 32(4), 641-670. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2021-0007 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2021-0007
Taylor, R. S., Francis, W. S., Borunda-Vazquez, L., & Carbajal, J. (2019). Mechanisms of word concreteness effects in explicit memory: Does context availability play a role?. Memory & cognition, 47(1), 169–181. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0857-x DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0857-x
Tornjanski, A. (2023). Efekat emocionalne valence na kognitivnu obradu konkretnih i apstraktnih reči u zadatku vođene reprodukcije [Emotional valence effect on cognitive processing of concrete and abstract words in cued recall task]. Master thesis, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade.
Troche, J., Crutch, S. J., & Reilly, J. (2017). Defining a Conceptual Topography of Word Concreteness: Clustering Properties of Emotion, Sensation, and Magnitude among 750 English Words. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 264438. https://doi.org/1.3389/fpsyg.2017.01787 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01787
Tse, C. S., & Altarriba, J. (2022). Independent effects of word concreteness and word valence on immediate serial recall. British Journal of Psychology, 113(3), 820-834. https://doi.org/1.1111/bjop.12566 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12566
Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S.-T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24(7), 1767–1777. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht025 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht025
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 820–838. https://doi.org/1.1037/0022-3514.76.5.820 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.76.5.820
Yao, B., Keitel, A., Bruce, G., Scott, G. G., O'Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2018). Differential emotional processing in concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(7), 1064. https://doi.org/1.1037/xlm0000464 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000464
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2025 Milica Popović Stijačić, Katarina Protić

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.